Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 8 de 8
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Neuron ; 112(9): 1487-1497.e6, 2024 May 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38447576

RESUMO

Little is understood about how engrams, sparse groups of neurons that store memories, are formed endogenously. Here, we combined calcium imaging, activity tagging, and optogenetics to examine the role of neuronal excitability and pre-existing functional connectivity on the allocation of mouse cornu ammonis area 1 (CA1) hippocampal neurons to an engram ensemble supporting a contextual threat memory. Engram neurons (high activity during recall or TRAP2-tagged during training) were more active than non-engram neurons 3 h (but not 24 h to 5 days) before training. Consistent with this, optogenetically inhibiting scFLARE2-tagged neurons active in homecage 3 h, but not 24 h, before conditioning disrupted memory retrieval, indicating that neurons with higher pre-training excitability were allocated to the engram. We also observed stable pre-configured functionally connected sub-ensembles of neurons whose activity cycled over days. Sub-ensembles that were more active before training were allocated to the engram, and their functional connectivity increased at training. Therefore, both neuronal excitability and pre-configured functional connectivity mediate allocation to an engram ensemble.


Assuntos
Medo , Neurônios , Optogenética , Animais , Camundongos , Neurônios/fisiologia , Neurônios/metabolismo , Medo/fisiologia , Região CA1 Hipocampal/fisiologia , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Masculino , Camundongos Endogâmicos C57BL , Condicionamento Clássico/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia
2.
Neuropsychopharmacology ; 48(5): 724-733, 2023 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36261624

RESUMO

Memories allow past experiences to guide future decision making and behavior. Sparse ensembles of neurons, known as engrams, are thought to store memories in the brain. Most previous research has focused on engrams supporting threatening or fearful memories where results show that neurons involved in a particular engram ("engram neurons") are both necessary and sufficient for memory expression. Far less is understood about engrams supporting appetitive or rewarding memories. As circumstances and environments are dynamic, the fate of a previously acquired engram with changing circumstances is unknown. Here we examined how engrams supporting a rewarding cue-cocaine memory are formed and whether this original engram is important in reinstatement of memory-guided behavior following extinction. Using a variety of techniques, we show that neurons in the lateral amygdala are allocated to an engram based on relative neuronal excitability at training. Furthermore, once allocated, these neurons become both necessary and sufficient for behavior consistent with recall of that rewarding memory. Allocated neurons are also critical for cocaine-primed reinstatement of memory-guided behavior following extinction. Moreover, artificial reactivation of initially allocated neurons supports reinstatement-like behavior following extinction even in the absence of cocaine-priming. Together, these findings suggest that cocaine priming after extinction reactivates the original engram, and that memory-guided reinstatement behavior does not occur in the absence of this reactivation. Although we focused on neurons in one brain region only, our findings that manipulations of lateral amygdala engram neurons alone were sufficient to impact memory-guided behavior indicate that the lateral amygdala is a critical hub region in what may be a larger brain-wide engram.


Assuntos
Complexo Nuclear Basolateral da Amígdala , Cocaína , Camundongos , Animais , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Neurônios/fisiologia , Cocaína/farmacologia
3.
Brain Res Bull ; 191: 61-68, 2022 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36279984

RESUMO

Memories of past experiences guide future behaviour. Sparse ensembles of neurons, known as engrams, are thought to store memories in the brain. Neurons involved in a particular engram ("engram neurons") are necessary for subsequent memory expression as memory retrieval is thought to be initiated by an external sensory cue reactivating engram neurons. However, conditions or environments are dynamic, such that future behaviour should be flexible. The role of engrams in mediating flexible behaviour is not understood. Here we examined this question using one type of flexible behaviour, extinction of a threat response. An initially neutral tone is first paired with an aversive footshock such that the tone alone induces defensive freezing. After subsequent repeated tone presentations without the footshock, rodents no longer freeze to the tone. Because the tone cue is thought to reactivate the engram to induce memory retrieval, we examined whether it is possible to induce an extinction-like behavioural effect by optogenetically reactivating the lateral amygdala component of the engram alone (without tone re-exposure). Similar to tone-induced extinction, mice showed decreased freezing to optogenetic stimulation of the lateral amygdala engram in the "extinction training" session. Moreover, "opto-extinguished" mice showed decreased freezing to the tone when subsequently tested for retrieval of the extinction training in the same context, suggesting that the opto-extinction transferred to the actual sensory stimulus. However, unlike tone extinction, in which mice showed renewal of tone-induced freezing when tested in a novel context, opto-extinguished mice continued to show a deficit in tone-induced freezing. Extinction has been characterized as new learning that inhibits the original memory or a phenomenon in which the original memory is "unlearned". Our findings suggest that opto-extinction may silence the original engram to "unlearn" the original memory.


Assuntos
Medo , Memória , Animais , Camundongos , Tonsila do Cerebelo/fisiologia , Extinção Psicológica/fisiologia , Medo/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Neurônios/metabolismo , Optogenética
4.
Front Neural Circuits ; 14: 42, 2020.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32792911

RESUMO

In vivo 1-photon (1p) calcium imaging is an increasingly prevalent method in behavioral neuroscience. Numerous analysis pipelines have been developed to improve the reliability and scalability of pre-processing and ROI extraction for these large calcium imaging datasets. Despite these advancements in pre-processing methods, manual curation of the extracted spatial footprints and calcium traces of neurons remains important for quality control. Here, we propose an additional semi-automated curation step for sorting spatial footprints and calcium traces from putative neurons extracted using the popular constrained non-negative matrixfactorization for microendoscopic data (CNMF-E) algorithm. We used the automated machine learning (AutoML) tools TPOT and AutoSklearn to generate classifiers to curate the extracted ROIs trained on a subset of human-labeled data. AutoSklearn produced the best performing classifier, achieving an F1 score >92% on the ground truth test dataset. This automated approach is a useful strategy for filtering ROIs with relatively few labeled data points and can be easily added to pre-existing pipelines currently using CNMF-E for ROI extraction.


Assuntos
Cálcio/metabolismo , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador/métodos , Aprendizado de Máquina , Microscopia de Fluorescência , Neurônios/metabolismo , Neurônios/patologia , Imagem Óptica , Automação , Humanos
5.
Neurobiol Learn Mem ; 174: 107284, 2020 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32745601

RESUMO

Memory is a constructive, not reproductive, process that is prone to errors. Errors in memory, though, may originate from normally adaptive memory processes. At the extreme of memory distortion is falsely "remembering" an event that did not occur. False memories are well-studied in cognitive psychology, but have received relatively less attention in neuroscience. Here, we took advantage of mechanistic insights into how neurons are allocated or recruited into an engram (memory trace) to generate a false memory in mice using only behavioral manipulations. At the time of an event, neurons compete for allocation to an engram supporting the memory for this event; neurons with higher excitability win this competition (Han et al., 2007). Even after the event, these allocated "engram neurons" remain temporarily (~6 h) more excitable than neighboring neurons. Should a similar event occur in this 6 h period of heightened engram neuron excitability, an overlapping population of neurons will be co-allocated to this second engram, which serves to functionally link the two memories (Rashid et al., 2016). Here, we applied this principle of co-allocation and found that mice develop a false fear memory to a neutral stimulus if exposed to this stimulus shortly (3 h), but not a longer time (24 h), after cued fear conditioning. Similar to co-allocation, the generation of this false memory depended on the post-training excitability of engram neurons such that these neurons remained more excitable during exposure to the neutral stimulus at 3 h but not 24 h. Optogenetically silencing engram neurons 3 h after cued fear conditioning impaired formation of a false fear memory to the neutral stimulus, while optogenetically activating engram neurons 24 h after cued fear conditioning created a false fear memory. These results suggest that some false memories may originate from normally adaptive mnemonic processes such as neuronal excitability-dependent allocation and memory linking.


Assuntos
Complexo Nuclear Basolateral da Amígdala/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Neurônios/fisiologia , Animais , Condicionamento Clássico , Medo , Feminino , Masculino , Camundongos Endogâmicos C57BL , Optogenética
6.
Neuron ; 107(4): 597-599, 2020 08 19.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32818473

RESUMO

Many brain areas represent aspects of learned behavior. How do representations differ between regions? In this issue of Neuron, Kyriazi et al. (2020) show how the amygdala and prefrontal cortex use distinct strategies to code features of a complex task.


Assuntos
Tonsila do Cerebelo , Córtex Pré-Frontal , Encéfalo , Neurônios
7.
Neuropsychopharmacology ; 45(6): 916-924, 2020 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31837649

RESUMO

The internal representation of an experience is thought to be encoded by long-lasting physical changes to the brain ("engrams") . Previously, we and others showed within the lateral amygdala (LA), a region critical for auditory conditioned fear, eligible neurons compete against one other for allocation to an engram. Neurons with relatively higher function of the transcription factor CREB were more likely to be allocated to the engram. In these studies, though, CREB function was artificially increased for several days before training. Precisely when increased CREB function is important for allocation remains an unanswered question. Here, we took advantage of a novel optogenetic tool (opto-DN-CREB) to gain spatial and temporal control of CREB function in freely behaving mice. We found increasing CREB function in a small, random population of LA principal neurons in the minutes, but not 24 h, before training was sufficient to enhance memory, likely because these neurons were preferentially allocated to the underlying engram. However, similarly increasing CREB activity in a small population of random LA neurons immediately after training disrupted subsequent memory retrieval, likely by disrupting the precise spatial and temporal patterns of offline post-training neuronal activity and/or function required for consolidation. These findings reveal the importance of the timing of CREB activity in regulating allocation and subsequent memory retrieval, and further, highlight the potential of optogenetic approaches to control protein function with temporal specificity in behaving animals.


Assuntos
Complexo Nuclear Basolateral da Amígdala , Optogenética , Animais , Medo , Memória , Camundongos , Neurônios
8.
Curr Protoc Neurosci ; 84(1): e51, 2018 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29944206

RESUMO

Miniaturized fluorescence microscopes for imaging calcium transients are a promising tool for investigating the relationship between behavior and population-level neuronal activity in rodents. However, commercially available miniature microscopes may be costly and, because they are closed source, may not be easily modified based on particular experimental requirements. Here, we describe how to build and use a low-cost compact head-mounted endoscope (CHEndoscope) system for in vivo calcium imaging. The CHEndoscope uses an implanted gradient index lens along with the genetically encoded calcium indicator GCaMP6 to image calcium transients from hundreds of neurons simultaneously in awake behaving mice. This system is affordable, open source, and flexible, permitting modification depending on the particular experiment. This article describes in detail the assembly, surgical implantation, data collection, and processing of calcium signals using the CHEndoscope system. The aim of this open framework is to provide an accessible set of miniaturized calcium imaging tools for the neuroscience research community. © 2018 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Cálcio/metabolismo , Endoscópios , Neuroimagem/instrumentação , Neurônios/metabolismo , Animais , Camundongos , Microscopia de Fluorescência/métodos , Vigília/fisiologia
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...